Just because you feel good
Doesn't make you right
Just because you feel good
Still want you here tonight.
Skunk Anansie, Hedonism
Prime Minister David Cameron thinks
that the Olympics will create a legacy for the whole of the UK and not
just London. But, true to his earlier statements about importance of
feelings and what-not, he warns that some of this legacy will be “hard to touch”.
The concept of 'hard to touch' is a strange
one in an era of evidence-based policy, in which, in the words of one civil
servant (presumably in thrall to the early philosophy of Wittgenstein), 'if I
can't measure it, it don't exist'. As you know, Wittgenstein
abandoned his early ideas and adopted a broader view, and perhaps Mr Cameron
has also been persuaded to take a more inclusive conception of reality. Or maybe he is just worried that the
only legacies of London 2012 will be immaterial.
Cameron's touchy-feeling tones when
discussing the Olympics are not without precedent. Consider this neat
piece of verbal gymnastics from New Labour's Game Plan policy
document, when it suggests that whilst the quantity of medals won at the
Olympics is of great importance, one must not neglect the “quality” of a
victory:
“‘Quality’ can be taken to be the extent to which victory produces the feelgood factor and national pride (as these are the main public benefits of high performance sport). If it is accepted that the more popular the sport, the greater the amount of feelgood which follows, then “quality” medals are those obtained in the most popular sports.”
So, according to Game Plan,
the feelgood factor is a quality that can be created and changed in amount
over time, and although it is admitted that it is “difficult to quantify”,
there seems little room for doubting that it can have a powerful causal effect.
So what is the feelgood factor, then?
In case you thought it was something to do with happiness, or
satisfaction, or well-being, or other philosophical fluff, the authors
of Game Plan tell us it can all be explained with hard science:
“The biological explanation for the feelgood sensation is due to the release of endorphin in the brain. Endorphin has been called the ‘happy hormone’ and is released for different reasons, mainly physical, but also from laughter and joy, experienced by, for example, your team winning a match. Endorphin helps to reduce pain and has even been found to enhance treatment of many illnesses and diseases.”
Remember that this piece of pseudo-scientific
woowoo comes from a government document; a document that is primarily
concerned with justifying the future of sport in the UK, and the billions of
pounds it says are needed to ensure it.
The feelgood factor has been repeatedly cited by governments and other agencies as one of the arguments for investment
in elite sport in general, and the London Olympics. In Game Plan, for example, it is listed as the first
of three 'virtuous outcomes' of elite sporting success (the others are economic
benefits and increased grass-roots participation).
If the Game Plan account is to be
accepted, the feelgood factor really means 'improved mood'. Perhaps you think that is a rather
feeble justification for close-to £10,000,000,000,000 expenditure from London
alone.
The weediness of the feelgood defence for
investment in elite sport is partly due to the fact that we can easily think of
more economical alternatives for generating good feelings and lifting our mood, from old episodes of Dad’s Army to Christmas with
family and friends, to a smile from a secret crush.
There is a second, more fatal
difficulty with the feelgood defence.
Improved mood following nice experiences is almost always short-lived. Humans are adaptive creatures (like all
creatures, in fact), and we readily adapt to changing environments. So improved mood or satisfaction
eventually results in a ‘hedonic treadmill’ by which the elevated state will
not continue, even if the circumstances that promote it are maintained.
In other words, the good mood that we hope
will come with the Olympic success will be short-lived, and will certainly last
a lot less time than it will take to pay off the debt from the Games.
And let’s not forget that moods – like
interest rates and skirt lengths – can go up as well as down. John Steele, former CEO of UK Sport,
spoke of the “the euphoria of a full Lord’s Cricket Ground … when a single
Ashes test was won”. But by the
same logic, any of England’s subsequent defeats would seem destined to result
in national despair. What will
happen to the feelgood factor if any of our Olympic hopefuls under-performs or
is injured?
If success in elite sport (let alone
second-hand, spectated success) has a potent positive effect on our mood,
presumably failure has the opposite.
And if this is true, then this is a real cause for concern, as the
nature of sports competition is that most players lose in the long-run.
These are not new arguments, nor particularly
scholarly. I am sure there are
those among the governments and their policy makers who know that the public
has been offered some pathetically weak rationale for their investment in the
Games. Personally, I’d prefer an
honest explanation like, ‘Hey, it’s the Olympics. We beat the French!’ I’d even take, ‘Look, I was a bit of a spotty geek at school; if we
get the Olympics, I get to hang out with Jessica Ennis and Victoria Pendleton!' I just wish they didn’t treat us like fools. It doesn't feel good at all.
No comments:
Post a Comment